Home What's New
Psychoanalytic Writings![](../../kleinpic.gif)
Psychotherapy Service Email Forums and Groups
Process Press Links |
Robert M. Young Online Writings
PSYCHOTIC ANXIETIES AND THE FADING HOPES OF THE LEFT
Robert M. Young
Where on earth is there a place where the left has an intact vision -
where, as Raymond Williams said he had taken for granted for most of his own adult life,
the welfare state is in place, and the only issue is the rate at which it will progress?
Where is socialism in the ascendant? Where can one hold forth a vision called 'communism'
without feeling foolish and out of date? Indeed, where can one feel sensible in holding
out a vision at all? These are not good times for a utopian version of the ego ideal, a
concept which, at the best of times, has been something of a Cinderella in psychoanalytic
metapsychology.
My conception of the ego ideal is that it is an agency which is the
repository of hope and transcendence, in contrast with the superego, which represents
transgression and punishment. A failure to meet its strictures induces guilt, while
failing to live up to the aspirations of the ego ideal induces shame. The superego is
punitive; the ego ideal positive. But, as any good sixties leftist knows, the categories
of any theory are themselves ideological, and there are eminent conservatives in the
psychoanalytic movement. I am thinking of Chasseguet-Smirgel (1985) and Grunberger (1979,
1989), who claim that the ego ideal never transcends infantile narcissism, depends on
shortcuts and a failure to negotiate the Oedipus complex and can never be realistic. This
position is explicitly anti-radical (Chasseguet-Smirgel and Grunberger, 1986; cf. Kovel,
1986). There are parallels here with Lenin (remember Lenin?), who characterised left-wing
communism (remember the libertarians of the seventies?) as 'a left-wing disorder' (Lenin,
1920). And that takes me, via the association of ideas, to John Lennon (who turns
out to be a not altogether worthy vessel, but who is?), whose death - it was already a
decade ago - raised in many of us with the poignant question, 'What happened to all that
hope?'
For most of us who took part in the critique of existing structures and
the movement toward alternative institutions - which Rudi Dutchke dubbed 'the long march
through the institutions' - what was most distressing of all was that our efforts were
undermined from within - by sectarianism, by 'the tyranny of structurelessness, by
self-envy when things went well. What was true of our struggles in a relatively
post-scarcity context was even more true of other long marches - the efforts in China,
Eastern Europe and the Third World to build some version of socialism. It is now, for the
most part, embarrassing to call oneself a marxist (and who would even think of calling
himself or herself a maoist?). The model of the market economy is in the ascendant, and
cynicism is rife.
In the face of all this, many people I know and respect have turned to
psychoanalysis as the last refuge of the disappointed revolutionary. I said in a recent
article that I knew a dozen or more people who had been prominent in the culture of left
periodicals who had gone into analysis and a number who had trained to be psychotherapists
or analysts (Young, 1992). In the New Statesman of a couple of weeks ago (21
February, p. 35) this was lampooned and ignorantly bracketed with inactivity, Lacan,
deconstruction and bean bags by David Widgery, an old Trot who has for the most part
fought the good fight but apparently undervalue the power of the inner world. I have had
this experience once before at the hands of a Trotskyist. I once asked C. L. R. James to
write an introduction to Joel Kovel's White Racism. He agreed and then reneged
after reading the book. He said that Marxism does not need a psychology and certainly not
psychoanalysis.
I am unrepentant about turning to psychoanalysis, both because it saved
me from despair and because some of the people from the libertarian left who have taken it
up seem to me to be doing fundamentally important work in reconceptualising the task of
the left - away from voluntarism, triumphalism and asceticism and toward a more realistic,
hedonic and principled approach to a politics for the long run. I am thinking of the work
of Barry Richards, Paul Hoggett (whose fine book, Partisans in an Uncertain World ,
is now at the printer's), Karl Figlio, Ann Scott, Joel Kovel, Margot Waddell, Victor
Wolfenstein, Mike Rustin, Margaret Rustin. What they and I have in common is the
interrogation of classic psychoanalytic texts to find the basis of our self-limitation and
the seeds of a new hope based on a deeper understanding of the balance between
constructive and destructive forces in human nature. I trust that I am right to conclude
that similar trajectories are involved in setting up the new programme here and in
convening this meeting.
You may feel that this is a bleak beginning, but it behoves us to know
what we are up against. Otherwise, as Bruce Springsteen says, there are things that'll
knock you down that you didn't see coming. I have never been in a group or institution in
which utterly bewildering and exhausting difficulties did not arise.
In an attempt to shed some light on all this, I want to gather together
and draw attention to the implications of Kleinian and neo-Kleinian ideas for how we think
of human nature, by which I mean, with respect to individuals and all other levels of
culture and civilisation. It turns out that defence against psychotic anxieties is offered
by Kleinians as a deeper explanation than the incest taboo as the basis of that thin and
all too easily breached veneer that constitutes civility and stands between what passes
for the social order, on the one hand, and chaos (or the fear of it), on the other. This
turns out to be a mixed blessing, since our defences against psychotic anxieties act as a
powerful brake on institutional and social change toward less rigid and more generous
relations between individuals and groups. Those of you who are familiar with this
literature will not hear anything new. My aim is to think as hard as I can about what it
would mean to take its conclusions seriously.
Freud's theory of civilisation drew attention to the taboo against
violent sexual competitiveness and rapaciousness as the corner-stone of civilisation. The
polymorphously sexual patriarch was said to have been killed by the primal horde, thus
establishing the incest taboo, the basis for all other taboos and the system of custom and
legality that gave birth to civilisation and culture, terms Freud refused to distinguish.
He constantly emphasised that 'man is a wolf to other men', that the veneer of
civilisation is thin and under threat from moment to moment and that all of life is a
constant struggle conducted in the fraught space between erotic and destructive instincts.
For Freud the basic conflicts occurred at this level of the psyche (see Young, 1992, ch.
2). As Meltzer describes it, Freud's world is 'a world of higher animals', 'creatures
seeking surcease from the constant bombardment of stimuli from inside and out'. He
contrasts Klein's world as 'one of holy babes in holy families plagued by the devils of
split off death instinct' (Meltzer, 1978, part 3, pp. 115-16).
This is not merely a difference of emphasis. The difference between the
worlds of Freud and Klein may also be described as one of level of explanation and
of causality. Bion put the point clearly in the conclusion to his essay, 'Group Dynamics -
A Re-view', which, as Menzies Lyth points out, was more explicit about the Kleinian
inspiration of his ideas than his better-known collection of essays, Experiences in
Groups. Bion says, 'Freud's view of the dynamics of the group seems to me to require
supplementing rather than correction' (Bion, 1955, p. 475). He accepts Freud's claim that
the family group is the basis for all groups but adds that 'this view does not go far
enough... I think that the central position in group dynamics is occupied by the more
primitive mechanisms which Melanie Klein has described as peculiar to the
paranoid-schizoid and depressive positions. In other words, I feel... that it is not
simply a matter of the incompleteness of the illumination provided by Freud's discovery of
the family group as the prototype of all groups, but the fact that this incompleteness
leaves out the source of the main emotional drives of the group' (ibid.). He then
summarises the notions of 'work group' and the 'basic assumptions' that assail them -
'dependence', 'pairing', 'fight-flight' - and suggests that these may have a common link
or may be different aspects of each other.
'Further investigation shows that each basic assumption contains
features that correspond so closely with extremely primitive part objects that sooner or
later psychotic anxiety, appertaining to these primitive relationships, is released. These
anxieties, and the mechanisms peculiar to them, have already been displayed in
psychoanalysis by Melanie Klein, and her descriptions tally well with the emotional
states' of the basic assumption group. Such groups have aims 'far different either from
the overt task of the group or even from the tasks that would appear to be appropriate to
Freud's view of the group as based on the family group. But approached from the angle of
psychotic anxiety, associated with phantasies of primitive part object relationships...
the basic assumption phenomena appear far more to have the characteristics of defensive
reactions to psychotic anxiety, and to be not so much at variance with Freud's views as
supplementary to them. In my view, it is necessary to work through both the stresses that
appertain to family patterns and the still more primitive anxieties of part object
relationships. In fact I consider the latter to contain the ultimate sources of all group
behaviour' (p. 476).
In Bion's view, then, what matters in individual and group behaviour is
more primitive than the Freudian level of explanation. The ultimate sources of our
distress are psychotic anxieties, and much of what happens in individuals and groups is a
result of defences erected against psychotic anxieties, so that we do not have to
endure them consciously.
I'll say something about the term 'psychotic' and then turn to the
concept of phantasy and the anxieties which primitive phantasies generate. To most of us
'psychotic' refers to psychosis, a primary disturbance of libidinal relations with
reality, and psychotic symptoms are an attempt to restore the link with objects (Laplanche
& Pontalis, p. 370). When I was trained as a psychiatric aide in a state mental
hospital in the 1950s, we were taught a small number of things about psychosis, and they
seemed adequate in those pre-Laing and pre-Goffman times. Psychotics were 'out of contact
with reality' for much or all of the time. They heard and saw things that were not there -
hallucinations - and wildly distorted things that were - delusions. The notion of
'psychotic' was safely restricted to the people designated as 'mad'.Their likely diagnoses
were schizophrenia (four varieties: catatonic, paranoid, hebephrenic, simple); true
paranoia; manic-depressive psychosis; psychotic depression; organic psychosis. The
categories of dementia praecox or schizophrenia and of manic-depressive psychosis have
been in existence for less then a century and are more recent than Freud and Breuer's Studies
on Hysteria. Emil Kraepelin coined the term 'dementia praecox' in 1896.
What we now call psychosis has always had a special place in
practically all cultures, although that place has varied from divine, to diabolical, to
providing special insight, to links with witchcraft and enviable freedom from social
(though not always physical) restraints. Think of the 'Ship of Fools' and the depiction
and expressions of the mad by Bosch, Breughel, Goya and van Gogh, Magritte and Man Ray, as
well as the manifests of the Surrealists and Dadaists. In their very different ways they
all celebrated illumination coming from the unconscious. Like the critiques of the
categories of psychiatry written by Foucault, Laing and Cooper, these artists pointed to
madness as offering a basis for making critiques of the repressions, sublimations and
alienation of conventional society and put one in touch with something truer and in some
senses better (see also Gordon, 1990). These notions remain widespread. In a recent BBC2
television film in a series on 'Madness', Jonathan Miller referred to ideas of the mad as
childlike, as direct beneficiaries of God and to the beatific association between poverty
and lunacy, while that morning's Observer (13 October 1991) alluded to 'the sixties
argument that the mad are truly sane'. I am not analysing or assessing these claims, only
noting their currency.
I want to turn now to the mechanisms in question in the Kleinian
tradition and their evolution from the asylum to the nursery. A very different picture of
the place and prevalence of psychotic processes is immediately apparent. Klein described
schizoid mechanisms as occurring 'in the baby's development in the first year of life
characteristically... the infant suffered from states of mind that were in all their
essentials equivalent to the adult psychoses, taken as regressive states in Freud's sense'
(Meltzer, 1978, part 3, p. 22). Klein says in the third paragraph of her most famous
paper, 'Notes on Some Schizoid Mechanisms', 'In early infancy anxieties characteristic of
psychosis arise which drive the ego to develop specific defence-mechanisms. In this period
the fixation-points for all psychotic disorders are to be found. This has led some people
to believe that I regard all infants as psychotic; but I have already dealt sufficiently
with this misunderstanding on other occasions' (Klein, 1975, vol. 3, p. 1). Meltzer
comments that 'Although she denied that this was tantamount to saying that babies are
psychotic, it is difficult to see how this implication could be escaped' (Meltzer, 1978,
part 3, p. 22).
Kleinian thinking evolved in three stages. As in the above quotation,
Klein saw schizoid mechanisms and the paranoid-schizoid position as fixation points,
respectively, for schizophrenia and manic-depressive psychosis. Then the paranoid-schizoid
and depressive positions became developmental stages. Her terminology included 'psychotic
phases', 'psychotic positions' and then 'positions' (Klein, 1975, vol. 1, pp. 275n-276n,
279). Thirdly, in the work of Bion and other post-Kleinians, these became economic
principles and part of the moment-to-moment vicissitudes of everyday life. The notations
'ps' (for paranoid-schizoid) and 'd' (for depressive) were connected with a double-headed
arrow - psd - to indicate how easily and frequently our inner states oscillate from the
one to the other and back again (Meltzer, 1978, part 3, p. 22). In Bion's writings on
schizophrenia an ambiguity remained as to whether the psychotic part of the personality is
ubiquitous or only present in schizophrenics. Meltzer concludes his exposition of Bion's
schizophrenia papers by referring to the existence of these phenomena in patients of every
degree of disturbance, even 'healthy' candidates in training (p.28). Going further, he and
colleagues have drawn on the inner world of autistic patients to illuminate the norm;
Frances Tustin has essayed on autistic phenomena in neurotic patients, while Sydney Klein
has described 'autistic cysts' in neurotic patients.
So much for bringing 'psychotic' into the realm of the normal and
neurotic. Turning now to 'phantasy', I'll begin by pointing out that a full page of the index to Developments in Pychoanalysis (Klein et al., 1952) is devoted to this single
term, and the entry fills half a page in the index of the historical account of The
Freud-Klein Controversies 1941-1945. The essays in Developments in Psychoanalysis are versions of the papers which formed the basis for that controversy. Many things
were at stake, but at the heart of it, in my opinion, was the question of the primacy of
the inner world, as opposed to the more interactive, adaptive framework of ideas which
came to be associated with ego psychology and, in our own time, 'contemporary
Freudianism'. Anna Freud rebuts the claim that she 'has an inveterate prejudice in favour
of the modes of external reality ... and of conscious mental processes' (King and Steiner,
1991, p. 328), but I think it is a legitimate demarcation between Kleinian and Freudian
orientations and became even more so at the hands of Hartmann, Kris, Lowenstein and the
American school epitomised by the systematising work of David Rapaport.
As a part of the issue over the primacy of the inner world, I believe
that people were genuinely spooked by the sheer craziness and nastiness of the inner world
as described by Klein and her supporters. Indeed, there is a protest along these lines by
Michael Balint, who drily comments in the discussion of Susan Isaacs' fundamentally
important paper (to which I shall turn next) that 'perhaps Mrs Klein is laying undue
emphasis on the role of hatred, frustration and aggression in the infant' (p. 347).
Fairbairn, in contrast, seemed to feel (at least at that time) that Kleinian accounts of
phantasy were so successfully descriptive of the inner world that he proposed dropping
'phantasy' in favour of 'inner reality' (p.359).
I begin with the elementary point that 'phantasy' refers
to 'predominantly or entirely unconscious phantasies', as distinct from the sort of
conscious fantasies or imaginings we associate with, for example,
Coleridge's explorations of the imagination (Isaacs, 1952, pp. 80-81). Joan Riviere
appeals to Freud's hypothesis that the psyche is always interpreting the reality of its
experiences - 'or rather, misinterpreting them - in a subjective manner that
increases its pleasure and preserves it from pain' (Riviere, 1952, p. 41). Freud calls
this process 'hallucination; and it forms the foundation of what we mean by phantasy-life.
The phantasy-life of the individual is thus the form in which the real internal and
external sensations and perceptions are interpreted and represented to himself in his mind
under the influence of the pleasure-pain principle'. Riviere adds that 'this primitive and
elementary function of his psyche - to misinterpret his perceptions for his own
satisfaction - still retains the upper hand in the minds of the great majority of even
civilised adults' (p.41). I suggest - and this lies at the heart of my paper - that this
point about misinterpreting the reality of the psyche's experience as normal and basic and
hallucinatory is the essential point - the ur-fact - about human nature. It is also the
essential basis for the theory of knowledge and our hopes for better human relations in
groups, institutions, communities and nations.
This general function for phantasy is repeated in Susan Isaacs'
definition. The '"mental expression" of instinct is the unconscious
phantasy...There is no impulse, no instinctual urge or response which is not experienced
as unconscious phantasy' (Isaacs, 1952, p. 83). The first mental processes... are to be
regarded as the earliest beginnings of phantasies. In the mental development of the
infant, however, phantasy soon becomes also a means of defence against anxieties, a means
of inhibiting and controlling instinctual urges and an expression of reparative wishes as
well... All impulses, all feelings, all modes of defence are experienced in phantasies
which give them mental life and show their direction and purpose' (ibid.).
When we turn to the content of the phantasies a problem of
communication arises: 'they are apt to produce a strong impression of unreality and
untruth' (Riviere, 1952, p. 20). This is because when we write or speak about them we are
clothing preverbal and very primitive mental processes in the language of words in
dictionaries. My way round this is to share some images and experience from my own
clinical and personal experience. Phantasies are rendered as black holes, nameless dread,
part objects, offal, shit, urine, a patients' dreams of wet cinders or barren desert
mindscapes, pus, slime, feelings of being overwhelmed, engulfed, disintegrated, in pieces,
devoured, falling through empty space, spiders, bugs, snakes. Language drawn from work
with autistic patients includes dread of falling apart, falling infinitely, spilling away,
exploding away, threat of total annihilation, unintegration (as distinct from
the disintegration of schizophrenia), experiencing a missing person as a hole (rather
than 'missing' them as not present).
When I cannot find a piece of paper or go to a room and cannot recall
why, I don't just think of age and preoccupation. The fabric of reality is rent asunder,
and I feel in imminent danger of dying, of disintegration, of unendurable panic. When I
was a boy there was a nearby grand house, set in large grounds in a gully, with walls and
a gate with a heavy chain and a wrought iron sign: 'Driverdale'. I could not go near it
without intense anxiety, so I didn't. (It was a feat of my adolescence to drive my
motor-bike at high speed through the grounds.) The same intense terror was experienced
with respect to a green house we had to pass on the way to the swimming pool. We called
the woman who lived there 'the green witch' and ran past. I believed in and feared the
Bogeyman and could not go to sleep unless the door of my wardrobe was shut. I was mortally
afraid of the Frankenstein monster and the Mummy (of 'The Mummy's Curse'), and until I
went away to university I could not go into the kitchen without first reaching round the
door jamb and turning on the fluorescent light, which took an age to go on. The same was
true of the back porch, while going into the back garden after dusk was simply out of the
question. My childhood and adolescence were filled with terrors, imaginings, fantasies and
some activities about which I would blush to tell - all rending the fabric of civilised
society. Prominent among the terrors was the sheer horror of hearing the word 'Terrell',
the name of the nearby state mental hospital. I cannot recall a time when this word did
not conjure up an unpicturable hell (later well- represented in the film 'Snake Pit'),
into which my depressed mother and I were in imminent danger of being tossed as a result
of my transgressions, in particular, my inability to be sufficiently respectful of my
father. A version of this terror still overcomes me when I am in the grip of an argument
and cannot let up. Behind these conscious experiences, I now know, lay psychotic
anxieties.
I offer these reports, somewhat shyly, as a way of inviting you to make
similar searches of your memories to glimpse the tips of the icebergs of your own
phantasies and psychotic anxieties. They are my version of what Klein calls 'a cave of
dangerous monsters' (Klein, vol. 1, p. 272). My general point is that if you ask the
question, 'What is a psychotic anxiety when it's at home and not in the pages of an
implausible and nearly unfathomable text by Melanie Klein?', you'll be able to be less
sceptical if you interrogate the fringes of your own memories and distressing experiences
and, of course, your dreams. I shall offer more illustrations anon, but for the present I
want to assert that psychotic anxieties are ubiquitous, underlie all thought, provide the
rationale for all culture and institutions and, in particular cases, help us to make sense
of especially galling ways of being. I have in mind at the moment Meltzer's idea of the
claustrum, wherein dwell ultra-ambitious and survivalist conformists who live in projective identification, which he takes to mean that their dwelling place in the inner
world is inside the rectum, thus confirming the colloquial description of such people as
'arseholes'. His analysis shows that this degree of use of projective identification is a
defence against schizophrenic breakdown. This suggests that many of our chief executives
and leaders live perpetually on the verge of madness. No wonder they absolutely must get
their way.
Klein's views on these matters are based on Freud and Abraham's notions
of oral libido and fantasies of cannibalism (Gedo, 1986, p. 94). She refers to sadistic
impulses against the mother's breast and inside her body, wanting to scoop out, devour,
cut to pieces, poison and destroy by every means sadism suggests (Klein, 1975, vol. 1, p.
262). Once again, the projective and introjective mechanisms of the first months and year
give rise to anxiety situations and defences against them, 'the content of which is
comparable to that of the psychoses in adults' (ibid.). Orality is everywhere, for
example, in the 'gnawing of conscience' (p. 268). Riviere says that 'such helplessness
against destructive forces within constitutes the greatest psychical danger-situation
known to the human organism; and that this helplessness is the deepest source of anxiety
in human beings' (Riviere, 1952, p. 43). It is the ultimate source of all neurosis. At
this early stage of development, sadism is at its height and is followed by the discovery
that loved objects are in a state of disintegration, in bits or in dissolution, leading to
despair, remorse and anxiety, which underlie numerous anxiety situations. Klein concludes,
'Anxiety situations of this kind I have found to be at the bottom not only of depression,
but of all inhibitions of work' (Klein, 1975 vol. 1, p. 270).
It should be recalled that these are pre-linguistic experiences
developmentally, and sub-linguistic in adults. It is a characteristic of the world view of
Kleinians that the primitive is never transcended and that all experiences continue to be
mediated through the mother's body. Similarly, there is a persistence of primitive
phantasies of body parts and bodily functions, especially biting, eating, tearing,
spitting out, urine and urinating, faeces and defecating, mucus, genitals. Powerful and
bewildering examples of these matters can be found in Klein's analyses of dreams and
children's play. Similarly daunting example could be drawn from Meltzer's account of the
dream materials which can be attributed to unconscious phantasies of anal masturbation
(Meltzer, 1988, esp. pp. 104, 106-7).
Here is an example of undiluted Klein. She is in the middle of an
exposition of the part which the paranoid, depressive and manic positions play in normal
development (Klein, 1975, vol. 1, p. 279) and offers two illustrative dreams, which I
shall not quote. (I should emphasise that I am quoting a passage from the middle of an
exposition and interpretation which is six pages long.) I want to convey the flavour of
the primitive phantasies which I have been discussing. Here is part of the interpretation:
'The urination in the dream led on to early aggressive phantasies of the patient towards
his parents, especially directed against their sexual intercourse. He had phantasied
biting them and eating them up, and among other attacks, urinating on and into his
father's penis, in order to skin and burn it and to make his father set his mother's
inside on fire in their intercourse (the torturing with hot oil). These phantasies
extended to babies inside his mother's body, which were to be killed (burnt). The kidney
burnt alive stood both for his father's penis - equated with faeces - and for the babies
inside his mother's body (the stove which he did not open). Castration of the father was
expressed by the associations about beheading. Appropriation of the father's penis was
shown by the feeling that his penis was so large and that he urinated both for himself and
for his father (phantasies of having his father's penis inside his own or joined on to his
own had come out a great deal in his analysis). The patient's urinating into the bowl
meant also his sexual intercourse with his mother (whereby the bowl and the mother in the
dream represented her both as a real and as an internalised figure). The impotent and
castrated father was made to look on at the patient's intercourse with his mother - the
reverse of the situation the patient had gone through in phantasy in his childhood. The
wish to humiliate his father is expressed by his feeling that he ought not to do so'
(Klein, vol. 1, p. 281). And so on for another half page.
Many Kleinians (though not all, for example, Meltzer) have altered
their language and have become more likely to make interpretations in terms of functions
rather than anatomical part objects. Edna O'Shaughnessy has suggested the notion of
'psychological part objects' as an analogy to bodily part objects. Spillius takes this up
and argues 'that we relate to psychological part objects... to the functions of the part
object rather than primarily to its physical structure. It is the capacities for seeing,
touching, tasting, hearing, smelling, remembering, feeling, judging, and thinking, active
as well as passive, that are attributed to and perceived in relation to part objects'.
Spillius concludes her remarks on this change in emphasis in technique by relating it to
Klein's concept of projective identification. The functions 'are frequently understood as
aspects of the self which are projected into part objects' (Spillius, 1988, vol. 1, pp.
2-5; cf. vol. 2, pp. 8-9).
This way of writing and representing our inner worlds is veritably hard
to bear, hard to credit, hard to follow. Klein and Meltzer are operating - well and truly
- in the most primitive parts of the inner world, where dream symbolism meets up with
primitive bodily functions and body parts. Their way of describing these phantasies is
easy to caricature and becomes wooden when adopted in a parrot-like fashion by
inexperienced acolytes. In the subsequent history of Kleinian psychoanalysis, however, her
outlook on unconscious phantasy has continued to prevail. Elizabeth Spillius reports that
this is one of Klein's concepts which has been 'very little altered' by subsequent
Kleinians (Spillius, 1988, vol. i, p. 2).
Klein was untroubled by being called an 'id psychologist' (Gedo, 1986,
p. 91). She unrepentantly conceived the analyst's task to be to confront the patient with
the content of the unconscious. She eschewed 'corrective emotional experience', did not
encourage regression and the reliving of infantile experiences, or explicit educational or
moral influences, and kept 'to the psycho-analytic procedure only, which, to put it in a
nutshell, consists in understanding the patient's mind and in conveying to him what goes
on in it' (Klein, 1975, vol. 3, p. 129). She felt that confidently articulating
interpretations of very primitive material in the face of resistance diminishes the
patient's anxiety and opens the door to the unconscious. Nor did she shy away from such
deep interpretations or transference interpretations from the beginning of analytic work
with a patient (Klein, 1975, vol. 2, pp. 22-24; Gedo, 1986, p. 92).
Why is all this such an innovation? Riviere points out that anxiety was
of great significance to Freud, but that much of his rhetoric was physiological. He did
not concern himself with the psychological content of phantasies. By contrast,
'Anxiety, with the defences against it, has from the beginning been Mrs Klein's approach
to psycho-analytical problems. It was from this angle that she discovered the existence
and importance of aggressive elements in children's emotional life... and [it] enabled her
to bring much of the known phenomena of mental disorder into line with the basic
principles of analysis' (Riviere, 1952, pp. 8-9).
From that point, Kleinians went on to propose elements of a general
psychology, including the claim that there is 'an unconscious phantasy behind every
thought and every act' (p.16). That is, the mental expression of primitive processes 'is unconscious phantasy' (ibid.). It is not only a background hum, as it were. Isaacs claims
that 'Reality thinking cannot operate without concurrent and supporting unconscious
phantasies' (Isaacs, 1952, p. 109). And again: 'phantasies are the primary content of
unconscious mental processes' (pp. 82, 112). 'There is no impulse, no instinctual urge or
response which is not experienced as unconscious phantasy' (p. 83). 'Phantasies have both
psychic and bodily effects, e.g., in conversion symptoms, bodily qualities, character and
personality, neurotic symptoms inhibitions and sublimations' (p. 112). They even determine
the minutiae of body language (p. 100). The role of unconscious phantasy extends from the
first to the most abstract thought. The infant's first thought of the existence of the
external world comes from sadistic attacks on the mother's body (Klein, 1975, vol. 1, p.
276; vol. 3, p. 5). 'Phantasies - becoming more elaborate and referring to a wider variety
of objects and situations - continue throughout development and accompany all activities;
they never stop playing a great part in mental life. The influence of unconscious phantasy
on art, on scientific work, and on the activities of everyday life cannot be overrated'
(Klein, 1975, vol. 3, p. 251; cf. p. 262).
These phantasies and the associated anxieties are not only ubiquitous:
they interact in complicated ways. As Riviere points out, 'It is impossible to do any
justice here to the complexity and variety of the anxiety-situations and the defences
against them dominating the psyche during these early years. The factors involved are so
numerous and the combinations and interchanges so variable. The internal objects are
employed against external, and external against internal, both for satisfaction and for
security; desire is employed against hate and destructiveness; omnipotence against
impotence, and even impotence (dependence) against destructive omnipotence; phantasy
against reality and reality against phantasy. Moreover, hate and destruction are employed
as measures to avert the dangers of desire and even of love. Gradually a progressive
development takes place... by means of the interplay of these and other factors, and of
them with external influences, out of which the child's ego, his object-relations, his
sexual development, his super-ego, his character and capacities are formed' (Riviere,
1952, pp. 59-60). You begin, I hope, to see the case for my title.
Turning to the bearings of these ideas on groups and institutions, I
want to begin with two points. The first is that the move is a simple one. Bion says, 'My
impression is that the group approximates too closely, in the minds of the individuals
composing it, to very primitive phantasies about the contents of the mother's body. The
attempt to make a rational investigation of the dynamics of the group is therefore
perturbed by fears, and mechanisms for dealing with them, which are characteristic of the
paranoid-schizoid position. The investigation cannot be carried out without the
stimulation and activation of those levels... the elements of the emotional situation are
so closely allied to phantasies of the earliest anxieties that the group is compelled,
whenever the pressure of anxiety becomes too great, to take defensive action' (Bion, 1955,
p. 456). The psychotic anxieties in question involve splitting and projective
identification and are characteristic of the paranoid-schizoid and depressive positions,
now as group processes (p.457). The move from the individual to the group does not raise
new issues about explanation. He says a little further on, 'The apparent difference
between group psychology and individual psychology is an illusion produced by the fact
that the group brings into prominence phenomena which appear alien to an observer
unaccustomed to using the group' (p. 461).
This is, at first glance, a shocking claim, but be in no doubt that it
is a consistent one in the history of psychoanalysis - that group and social phenomena can
and should be explained on the same principles as individual phenomena. Freud simply did
not grant a separate conceptual space for social science. He wrote in the New
Introductory Lectures , 'Strictly speaking there are only two sciences: psychology,
pure and applied, and natural science' (Freud, 1933, p. 179). And again: 'the events in
human history, the interactions between human nature, cultural developments and the
precipitates of primeval experience... are only the reflection of the dynamic conflicts
among id, ego and superego, which psychoanalysis studies in the individual - the same
events repeated on a wider stage' (quoted in Gay, 1988, p. 547). His biographer comments,
'He could not have stated the essential unity of his thought any more forcefully' (ibid.).
I believe that the shocking reductionism apparently involved in this claim is mitigated
and probably on its way to being eliminated by developments in the wake of Bion's later,
strictly psychoanalytic, work. Rather than lament the difficulties in articulating
psychoanalytic concepts with ones (which ones?) in social and political discourse, it may
very well prove more fruitful to delve, as Bion did, further into the individual psyche.
There we find group and institutional and social structures hard at work - in what
Rosenfeld called 'the gang in the mind' (Rosenfeld, 1988, esp. p. 249), in what David
Armstrong calls 'the institution in the mind' (Armstrong, 1991),and in what a number of
writers have described as 'pathological organizations' (Spillius, 1988, Part 4). I suggest
that this is a fundamentally important growth point in the psychoanalytic understanding of
groups and organizations and that these notions are beginning to shed floods of light on
why they are so hard to change. It's another perspective on just how primitive and
refractory the domain Bion designated 'basic assumption functioning' is.
My second point is that those of us who have tried to change
institutions will be relieved to have this illumination and to be better informed about
what we are up against. I remember with some chagrin the occasion when Bob Hinshelwood
(who has since published the best book on groups) insisted that I train in group therapy
and go to a two-week residential Leicester Conference on group relations (Miller, 1990). I
was offended by his saying I'd had no experience of groups, since I'd spent my Sixties and
Seventies in all sorts of collectives, co-ops and even a commune. I felt he was being
dismissive of some of my most painful scar tissue, and we had a blazing row about which of
us was being arrogant... Looking back from the vantage point of a number of years of
conducting and being supervised on group therapy, as well as trying to assimilate the
experience of a Leicester Conference (which all acknowledge takes years), I gratefully
(and only residually resentfully) say that unless we understand the psychotic anxieties
Bion is on about, we will never know the magnitude of the task and will be bound to fail.
Bion says that falling into the forms of basic assumption functioning which he describes
is instinctive, involuntary, automatic, instantaneous and inevitable (pp. 449, 458).
Elliott Jaques and Isabel Menzies Lyth are also very sober and stoical
in their assessments of the barriers to change. Jaques begins his essay on 'Social Systems
as a Defence against Persecutory and Depressive Anxiety' by reiterating that 'social
phenomena show a striking correspondence with psychotic processes in individuals', that
'institutions are used by their individual members to reinforce individual mechanisms of
defence against anxiety', and 'that the mechanisms of projective and introjective
identification operate in linking individual and social behaviour'. He argues the thesis
that 'the primary cohesive elements binding individuals into institutionalised human
association is that of defence against psychotic anxiety' (Jaques, 1955, pp. 478-9). He
points out that the projective and introjective processes he is investigating are basic to
even the most complex social processes and directs us to Paula Heimann's argument that
they are at the bottom of all our dealings with one another (p. 481, 481n). His conclusion
is cautionary and points out the conservative - even reactionary - consequences of our
psychotic anxieties and our group and institutional defences against them. He suggests
that as a result of these reflections on human nature 'it may become more clear why social
change is so difficult to achieve, and why many social problems are so intractable. From
the point of view here elaborated, changes in social relationships and procedures call for
a restructuring of relationships at the phantasy level, with a consequent demand upon
individuals to accept and tolerate changes in their existing patterns of defences against
psychotic anxiety. Effective social change is likely to require analysis of the common
anxieties and unconscious collusions underlying the social defences determining phantasy
social relationships' (p.498).
I turn, penultimately, to the investigator whose work strikes me as the
most important body of writings on the social bearings of psychoanalysis, Isabel Menzies
Lyth, who built her research on the shoulders of Bion and Jaques. (She was in a group with
Bion, and he was her analyst.) She has investigated a number of fraught settings, for
example, the fire brigade, motor-cycling, children's institutions, as well as a number of
industrial ones, and most recently the tripartite group structure of the Institute of
Psychoanalysis in London.
The piece of research which has deservedly made her world-famous is
described in a report entitled 'The Functioning of Social Systems as a Defence against
Anxiety'. It is a particularly poignant document, which addresses the question why people
of good will and idealistic motives do not accomplish what they intend, that is, why
nurses find themselves, to an astonishing degree, not caring for patients and leaving the
nursing service in droves. It would be repetitious to review the mechanisms she describes.
They are the ones I have been discussing. What is so distressing is that they operate
overwhelmingly in a setting which has as its very reason for existence the provision of
sensitivity and care. Yet that setting is full of threats to life itself and arouses the
psychotic anxieties I have outlined. She says, 'The objective situation confronting the
nurse bears a striking resemblance to the phantasy situations that exist in every
individual in the deepest and most primitive levels of the mind. The intensity and
complexity of the nurse's anxieties are to be attributed primarily to the peculiar
capacity of objective features of her work situation to stimulate afresh those early
situations and their accompanying emotions' (Menzies Lyth, 1988, pp. 46-7).
The result is the evolution of socially structured defence mechanisms
which take the form of routines and division of tasks which effectively preclude the nurse
relating as a whole person to the patient as a whole person. 'The implicit aim of such
devices, which operate both structurally and culturally, may be described as a kind of
depersonalisation or elimination of individual distinctiveness in both nurse and patient.
For example, nurses often talk about patients not by name, but by bed numbers or by their
diseases or a diseased organ: "the liver in bed 10" or "the pneumonia in
bed 15". Nurses deprecate this practice, but it persists' (pp. 51-2). She lists and
discusses the reifying devices which reduce everyone involved to part-objects, including
insight into why the nurse wakes you up to give you a sleeping pill (p.69). There is a
whole system of overlapping ways of evading the full force of the anxieties associated
with death, the ones which lie at the heart of the mechanisms which Klein described (pp.
63-64; cf. Riviere, 1952, p. 43).
Menzies Lyth also draws a cautionary conclusion: 'In general, it may be
postulated that resistance to social change is likely to be greatest in institutions whose
social defence systems are dominated by primitive psychic defence mechanisms, those which
have been collectively described by Klein as the paranoid-schizoid defences' (Menzies
Lyth, p. 79). In recent reflections on her work and that of her colleagues, she has
reiterated just how refractory to change institutions are (Menzies Lyth, 1988, pp. 1-42,
and personal communications). It is obvious to me that these findings apply across the
society and culture and to left organizations particularly, where the risks of going
against the grain of hegemony can often feel life-threatening and in some societies are.
The Leicester Conferences on group and organisational behaviour, with
particular emphasis on authority and leadership, have been held at least once a year since
1957. They are heir to the traditions discussed above, especially the work of Klein, Bion,
Jaques and Menzies Lyth. (Other influences are mentioned in Miller, 1990, pp. 165-69.) One
among several interrelated ways of characterising the two-week residential conferences is
that they are so arranged as to facilitate experiential learning about the ways in which
group processes can generate psychotic anxieties and institutional defences against them
(p. 171). The struggles that ensue in the members' minds between individuation and
incorporation, as a result of the conference group events, is hard to credit by anyone who
has not taken part in a Leicester Conference or related 'mini-Leicester' events.
Similarly, descriptions of occcurrences and feelings are likely to seem odd to anyone not
familiar with the sorts of activities around which the conferences are structured. I
believe, however, that the relevant emotional points will be sufficiently clear without a
(necessarily) long description of the conference rubric.
My own experience involved feeling continually on the edge of
disintegration as a result of what happened in the various group events (ranging in size
from a dozen to over 100 people) which I found appalling and from which there seemed no
escape, while efforts to persuade people to behave well produced flight, sadism collusive
lowering of the stakes or denial. The potential of the group for uniting around (what was
called on occasion) 'cheap reconciliation' or for cruelty, brought me to the point of
leaving at several points, and I frequently had the experience of having to use all my
resources to hold myself together against forces which I experienced as profoundly
immoral, amoral or pathetically conformist. No appeal to standards of group decency was of
much avail.
I ended up forming a group in my mind which consisted of all the people
I admired in history and in my lifetime, e.g., Socrates, Lincoln, Gandhi, King,
Bonhoeffer, Marcuse, Mandela, who had stood up to intolerable social forces without
quitting the field or having their spirits broken. I dubbed this 'The psd Solidarity
Group' and, armed with their mandate, managed to talk my way into a meeting with the
staff, for the purpose of mounting a critique of the rubric of the exercise. I felt
contained by the inner solidarity provided by my imagined group, while I was, in truth,
actually on my own in the phenomenal context of the conference events. I had blown out of
a group in considerable distress, because it had utterly failed to live up to its
self-designation of advocating and practising decency and civility among its members and
urging such standards on the larger group of conference members.
Just as I was on the point of sitting down to confront the staff group
in the name of my inner world group (vainly hoping they would show some interest in its
name, membership and values), a representative of the group I had left appeared and
bestowed 'plenipotentiary powers' (one of the designated forms of delegation of authority)
on me, freeing me from the dreaded status of 'singleton'. A singleton is a person with no
role status in he large group (see Miller, 1990, p. 179 and Turquet, 1975, where the
plight of the singleton is insightfully and poignantly described). I had felt unutterably
alone, almost totally in the grip of paranoid persecutions, holding on for dear life to my
hallucinated historical group. The bestowal of my conference group's trust reincorporated
me into the social whole on terms I could accept.
My confrontation with the staff group, acting in this exercise as
'Management, was predictably without issue, but I went away feeling that I had spoken my
piece without suffering the humiliation that many others had experienced. I had offered my
analysis of the situation and their role in it, one dimension of which was that they would - as a part of the exercise's point - continue to behave as that were doing, i.e., act as
an immovable object on to which the groups would project their phantasies about authority
and (hopefully) begin to take responsibility for themselves. I felt that I had done that
and negotiated my own rite of passage - just.
Having gone some way toward resolving my own temporary insanity (though
not my omnipotence or my paranoia, which included the belief that the conference Director
had slept with my partner) I was only able to bask pleasantly in group membership for a
few minutes before members of another group, who had sought refuge in being regressed and
silly (they called themselves 'The Potty Training Group'), stormed into the room where the
staff/Management group were holding court. The person whom I had considered to be the
mildest member of that group proceeded to physically attack a German member of staff with
shouts of 'fascist' and other violent epithets. He was aided and cheered on by other
members of his group, until one, a woman I felt sure was a Jew but I now recollect was
probably not but was a German, broke down sobbing and shouted for all this to stop.
The descent from work or task-oriented groups to groups in the thrall
of psychotic basic assumptions is, as Bion pointed out, spontaneous and inevitable, even
in a situation which all concerned know to be temporary and 'artificial'. I continue to
find this profoundly sobering. I also continue to ruminate it and am far from having
digested the experience, though I have found it increasingly helpful in my work and
related activities - and in my reflections on what has happened to the left,especially the
left which tried to work in relatively non-hierarchical groups.
After canvassing the literature on psychotic anxieties and reflecting
on it and my own personal, clinical and politicalexperience, I am left with a daunting
sense of the power of the inner world and an awesome awareness of how very deep,
primitive, abiding and alarming its nether regions are. The anxieties I have attempted to
outline (and, to a degree, evoke), exist throughout human nature - in all of life from the
cradle (some say earlier) to the grave, in all of play and culture, and act as a brake on
benignity and social change which it is hard to imagine releasing, even notch by notch.
The history of psychoanalysis has left us with a small number of ideas
about the veneer of civilisation. Freud said it was thin and under threat. One reading of
those who still speak in his name and quote his slogan: 'Where id was, there ego shall be.
It is a work of culture - not unlike the draining of the Zuider Zee' (S.E. 22, p.
80), takes this to mean that the result can be a dry, flowering land, i.e., that there can
be a 'conflict-free sphere of the ego'. A second, rather disparate, group proffer a
continuum extending from Reich's advocacy of desublimation and a promise of a return to
Eden, to the Winnicottian position that eschews Klein's undoubted stress on the power of
thantic, destructive forces, and sees rather more decency and hope in liberal society than
the Kleinians discern.
I dare say that Klein wrote rather less about the other side of human
nature - the constructive or erotic impulses - because she found herself in mutually
critical dialogue with colleagues who she felt overemphasised those aspects. Finding the
twig bent, as she thought, too far one way, she bent it the other way, perhaps to leave it
straight for those that followed. A third group are orthodox Kleinians and point out that
the veneer of civilisation is very thin indeed and that the maelstrom beneath is
perpetually and rather pathetically defended against. It can be argued that this provides
the basis for a psychoanalytic rendering of Gramsci's optimism of the will, coupled with a
pessimism of the intellect and, I say again, a belief that it is essential to know what is
bubbling away underneath the surface if we are to have any hope of cooling some of the
crust and sharing some of the fruits of human endeavour more equtably.
I also believe that this position is consistent with a careful reading
of Freud's Civilisation and Its Discontents, written half way through his
sixteen-year struggle with cancer. He says there that the history of civilisation is 'the
struggle between Eros and Death, between the instinct of life and the instinct of
destruction, as it works itself out in the human species. This struggle is what all life
essentially consists of... And it is this battle of the giants that our nurse-maids try to
appease with their lullaby about Heaven' (S.E. 21, p. 122).
In conclusion, my view of the connection between psychoanalysis,
therapy and social change is that human nature is far more ambivalent and refractory at a
much deeper level than we ever imagined when we embarked on changing the world. As I have
said before, I find myself thinking increasingly of Sisyphus, whom Camus urged us to
imagine as happy. Perhaps he comforts himself with the stoical maxim: 'It is not given to
us to complete the task, yet we may not give it up'.
This is a modified and extended version of a talk delivered at a
conference on 'Psychoanalysis, Psychotherapy and Social Change' at Manchester Polytechnic,
7 March l992.
REFERENCES (Place of publication
is London unless otherwise specified.)
Armstrong, D. (1991) 'The "Institution in the Mind":
Reflections in the Relation of Psycho-analysis to Work with Institutions', paper read to
the conference on Psychoanalysis and the Public Sphere, Polytechnic of East London,1-2
November.
Bion, W. R. (1955) 'Group Dynamics - a Re-view', in Klein et al., eds.
(1955), pp. 440-77.
______ (1961) Experiences in Groups and Other Papers. Tavistock.
______ (1967) Second Thoughts: Selected Papers on Psycho-Analysis.
Heinemann; reprinted Karnac, 1984.
Breuer, J. and Freud, S. (1895) Studies on Hysteria. S.E. 2, pp. 1-251.
Chasseguet-Smirgel, J. (1985) The Ego Ideal: A Psychoanalytic Essay
on the Malady of the Ideal. Free Association Books.
______ and Grunberger, B. (1986) Freud or Reich? Psychoanalysis and
Illusion. Free Association Books.
Cooper, D. (1972) The Death of the Family. Harmondsworth:
Penguin.
Figlio, K. (1990) 'The Environment: Topographiess of the Internal and
External Worlds', talk delivered to Centre for Psychoanalytic Studies, University of Kent.
Foucault, M. (1967) Madness and Civilisation: A History of Insanity
in the Age of Reason. Tavistock.
Freud, S. (1953-73) The Standard Edition of the Complete
Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, 24 vols. Hogarth. (S.E.)
______(1930) Civilization and Its Discontents. S.E. 21,
pp. 59-145.
______(1933) New Introductory Lectures on Psychoanalysis, S. E. 22, pp. 1-182.
Gay, P. (1988) Freud: A Life for Our Time. Dent.
Gedo, J. E. (1986) Conceptual Issues in Psychoanalysis: Essays in
History and Method. New York: Analytic Press.
Gordon, Colin (1990) 'Histoire de la folie: An Unknown Book by
Michel Foucault', History of the Human Sciences 3:3-26 (with several responses, pp.
27-67).
Grunberger, B. (1979) Narcissism: Psychoanalytic Essays. N. Y.:
International Universities Press.
______(1989) New Essays on Narcissism. Free Association Books.
Hartmann, H. (1939) Ego Psychology and the Problem of Adaptation.
New York: International Universities Press.
______. Kris, E. and Lowenstein, R. M. (1946) 'Comments on the
Formation of Psychic Structure', The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 2:11-38.
Hinshelwood, R.D. (1987) What Happens in Groups: Psychoanalysis, the
Individual and the Community. Free Association Books.
Hoggett, P. (1992) Partisans in an Uncertain World. Free
Association Books.
Isaacs, S. (1952) 'The Nature and Function of Phantasy', in Klein et
al. (1952), pp. 67-121.
Jaques, E. (1955) 'Social Systems as Defence against Persecutory and
Depressive Anxiety', in Klein et al., eds. (1955), pp. 478-98.
King, P. & Steiner, R. (1991) The Freud-Klein Controversies
1941-45. Tavistock/Routledge.
Klein, M. (1975) The Writings of Melanie Klein, 4 vols. Hogarth.
______ et al. (1952) Developments in Psycho-Analysis. Hogarth;
reprinted Karnac, 1989.
______ et al., eds. New Directions in Psycho-Analysis: The
Significance of Infant Conflict in the Patterns of Adult Behaviour. Tavistock;
reprinted Maresfield Library, 1977.
Klein, S. (1980) 'Autistic Phenomena in Neurotic Patients', Int. J.
Psycho-Anal. 61:395-402.
Kovel, J. (1970) White Racism: A Psychohistory. N. Y.: Pantheon; reprinted Free Association Books, 1988.
______(1986) 'Why Freud or Reich?' Free Assns. 4:80-99.
Laing, R.D. (1970) The Divided Self. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Laplanche, J. and Pontalis, J.- B. (1973) The Language of
Psycho-Analysis. Hogarth.
Lenin, V. I. (1920) 'Left-Wing' Communism, An Infantile Disorder. N. Y. : International Publishers, 1940.
Meltzer, D. (1978) The Kleinian Development, Parts 1-3. Strath
Tay: Clunie.
______ (1988) 'The Relation of Anal Masturbation to Projective
Identification', in Spillius (1988), vol. 1, pp. 102-16.
______ (1991) Lecture on projective identification and the claustrum
(tape).
______ et al. (1975) Explorations in Autism: A Psycho-Analytical
Study. Clunie.
Menzies Lyth, I. (1988) 'The Functioning of Social Systems as a Defence
against Anxiety' (1951), in Containing Anxiety in Institutions: Selected Essays, Vol. 1.
Free Association Books, pp. 43-85.
Miller, E. (1990) 'Experiential Learning Groups I: The Development of
the Leicester Model', in E. Trist and H. Murray, eds., The Social Engagement of Social
Science: A Tavistock Anthology. Vol. 1: The Socio-Psychological Perspective. Free
Association Books, pp. 165-85.
Rapaport, D. (1960) The Structure of Psychoanalytic Theory: A
Systematizing Attempt. Psychological Issues 2/2, Monograph 6. New York:
International Universities Press.
Richards, B. , ed. (1984) Capitalism and Infancy: Essays on
Psychoanalysis and Politics. Free Association Books.
______, ed. (1989) Crises of the Self: Further Essays on
Psychoanalysis and Politics. Free Association Books.
Riviere, J. (1952) 'General Introduction', in Klein et al. (1952), pp.
1-36.
______ (1952) 'On the Genesis of Psychical Conflict in Early Infancy',
in Klein et al. (1952), pp. 37-66.
Rosenfeld, H. (1971) 'A Clinical Approach to the Psychoanalytic Theory
of the Life and Death Instincts: An Investigation into the Aggressive Aspects of
Narcissism', Int. J. Psycho-Anal. 52:169-78, reprinted in Spillius, 1988, vol. 1,
pp. 239-55.
Rustin, M. (1991) The Good Society and the Inner World:
Psychoanalysis, Politics and Culture. Verso.
______ and Rustin, M. (1987) Narratives of Love and Loss: Studies in
Modern Children's Fiction. Verso.
Scott, Ann (1990) 'Melanie Klein and the Questions of Feminism', Women. 1:127-34.
Spillius, E. (1988) Melanie Klein Today, 2 vols. Routledge.
Turquet, P. (1975) 'Threats to Identity in the Large Group', in L.
Kreeger, ed., The Large Group: Dynamics and Therapy. Constable; reprinted
Maresfield Library, n.d., pp.87-144.
Tustin, F. (1986) Autistic Barriers in Neurotic Patients.
Karnac.
Waddell, M. and Williams, G. (1991) 'Reflections on Perverse States of
Mind', Free Assns. (no.22) 2:203-13.
Wolfenstein, E. V. (1990) 'Group Phantasies and 'the Individual', Free Assns. 20:150-80.
______(1991) 'On the Uses and Abuses of Psychoanalysis in Cultural
Research', Free Assns. (no.24) 2:515-47.
Young, R.M. (1992) Mental Space. Process Press, forthcoming.
Address for correspondence: 26 Freegrove Rd., London N7 9RQ
robert@rmy1.demon.co.uk
© The Author
|
|